Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About BiO
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contact
    • Contact BiO
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

User menu

  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
Biology Open
  • COB
    • About The Company of Biologists
    • Development
    • Journal of Cell Science
    • Journal of Experimental Biology
    • Disease Models & Mechanisms
    • Biology Open

supporting biologistsinspiring biology

Biology Open

Advanced search

RSS   Twitter   Facebook   YouTube

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Accepted manuscripts
    • Issue in progress
    • Latest complete issue
    • Issue archive
    • Archive by article type
    • Interviews
    • Sign up for alerts
  • About us
    • About BiO
    • Editors and Board
    • Editor biographies
    • Grants and funding
    • Journal Meetings
    • Workshops
    • The Company of Biologists
    • Journal news
  • For authors
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Aims and scope
    • Presubmission enquiries
    • Article types
    • Manuscript preparation
    • Cover suggestions
    • Editorial process
    • Promoting your paper
    • Open Access
  • Journal info
    • Journal policies
    • Rights and permissions
    • Media policies
    • Reviewer guide
    • Sign up for alerts
  • Contact
    • Contact BiO
    • Advertising
    • Feedback
Research Article
Role of water flow regime in the swimming behaviour and escape performance of a schooling fish
Lauren E. Nadler, Shaun S. Killen, Paolo Domenici, Mark I. McCormick
Biology Open 2018 7: bio031997 doi: 10.1242/bio.031997 Published 19 October 2018
Lauren E. Nadler
1ARC Centre of Excellence for Coral Reef Studies, James Cook University, Townsville, Queensland 4811, Australia
2Department of Marine Biology and Aquaculture, James Cook University, Townsville, Queensland 4811, Australia
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Lauren E. Nadler
  • For correspondence: lauren.e.nadler@gmail.com
Shaun S. Killen
3Institute of Biodiversity, Animal Health and Comparative Medicine, University of Glasgow, Glasgow G12 8QQ, Scotland, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Paolo Domenici
4CNR-IAMC, Istituto per l'Ambiente Marino Costiero, Localita Sa Mardini, Torregrande, 09170, Oristano, Italy
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Mark I. McCormick
1ARC Centre of Excellence for Coral Reef Studies, James Cook University, Townsville, Queensland 4811, Australia
2Department of Marine Biology and Aquaculture, James Cook University, Townsville, Queensland 4811, Australia
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & tables
  • Supp info
  • Info & metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF + SI
  • PDF
Loading

ABSTRACT

Animals are exposed to variable and rapidly changing environmental flow conditions, such as wind in terrestrial habitats and currents in aquatic systems. For fishes, previous work suggests that individuals exhibit flow-induced changes in aerobic swimming performance. Yet, no one has examined whether similar plasticity is found in fast-start escape responses, which are modulated by anaerobic swimming performance, sensory stimuli and neural control. In this study, we used fish from wild schools of the tropical damselfish Chromis viridis from shallow reefs surrounding Lizard Island in the Great Barrier Reef, Australia. The flow regime at each site was measured to ascertain differences in mean water flow speed and its temporal variability. Swimming and escape behaviour in fish schools were video-recorded in a laminar-flow swim tunnel. Though each school's swimming behaviour (i.e. alignment and cohesion) was not associated with local flow conditions, traits linked with fast-start performance (particularly turning rate and the distance travelled with the response) were significantly greater in individuals from high-flow habitats. This stronger performance may occur due to a number of mechanisms, such as an in situ training effect or greater selection pressure for faster performance phenotypes in areas with high flow speed.

This article has an associated First Person interview with the first author of the paper.

INTRODUCTION

Environmental flow conditions (e.g. wind in terrestrial habitats and currents in aquatic systems) can be variable and rapidly changing in many habitat types (Madin et al., 2006; McLaren et al., 2014). For associated animal communities, variability in flow adds a level of complexity to activities such as foraging and navigation, particularly for animals that fly or swim (Krupczynski and Schuster, 2008; Riley et al., 1999; Srygley, 2001; Thorup et al., 2003). This additional challenge is the result of drift, in which animals must compensate for downstream displacement in order to effectively engage in essential activities (McLaren et al., 2014). In complex marine habitats, water flow patterns are influenced by wind, weather and tide conditions as well as the bathymetry of the benthos (Johansen, 2014; Madin et al., 2006; Nikora, 2010; Poff et al., 1997). In this era of rapidly changing climates, storm frequency and intensity are likely to increase in the future (Huntington, 2006), potentially changing temporal and spatial water flow patterns and breaking down structural complexity (Lilley and Schiel, 2006; Madin and Connolly, 2006). Acute high-flow events could present problems for animal assemblages, as the behaviour and physiology of resident animals are likely suited to their habitat's original conditions (Fulton and Bellwood, 2005; Johansen et al. 2007; Munks et al., 2015; Nunes et al., 2013).

Many fishes use group living (e.g. schooling) as a mechanism that may reduce energy costs associated with swimming (Abrahams and Colgan, 1985; Herskin and Steffensen, 1998; Marras et al., 2015; Weihs, 1973). Schooling is widespread among fish species and carries benefits for individuals with respect to predator avoidance, foraging opportunities and energy use (Krause and Ruxton, 2002; Nadler et al., 2016; Shaw, 1978). However, these benefits depend on how well the members of a school can coordinate their behaviours (Handegard et al., 2012). To maximise the benefits of grouping, schools exhibit plasticity in behavioural traits in response to individual needs and environmental stimuli, particularly in group cohesion, coordination and positional preferences (Hansen et al., 2015; Killen et al., 2012; Krause and Ruxton, 2002; Sogard and Olla, 1997; Ward and Webster, 2016; Webster et al., 2007). Environmental conditions such as water flow regime can influence behavioural and physiological phenotypes of both solitary and schooling fish (Anwar et al., 2016; Binning et al., 2015; Langerhans, 2008; Liao, 2007; West-Eberhard, 1989). Chicoli et al. (2014) found that individuals and schools exhibit a greater rate of reaction to a threat under an acute high flow treatment compared to a no-flow treatment. In wild-caught fish, Binning et al. (2014) found that fish from wave-exposed (and hence higher flow) sites exhibited greater aerobic swimming performance than individuals from sheltered (lower flow) sites.

The fast-start escape response is one of the main forms of defence used by fish against a predator that has initiated a strike. This response consists of a rapid, anaerobically-fuelled acceleration typically mediated by a pair of higher order command neurons called Mauthner cells (M-cell) in response to threatening sensory stimuli (Domenici, 2010; Korn and Faber, 2005). Hence, this behaviour is modulated by anaerobic swimming performance, sensory abilities and neural control. This type of response typically occurs on the order of milliseconds and is generally divided into three stages for the purpose of comparative analysis: stage 1 ­– unilateral muscle contraction on the side of the body opposite to the stimulus, causing the fish to bend into a C shape; stage 2 – contralateral muscle contraction, causing the tail to flip around creating additional forward acceleration; and stage 3 ­– variable stage with fish either gliding or burst swimming (Domenici and Blake, 1997; Tytell and Lauder, 2008; Wakeling, 2005). Anaerobic swimming performance in particular may be fundamental to survival under high flow conditions. Anaerobic swimming is characterised by burst-type swimming, powered by fast, glycolytic white muscle (Olson, 1998; Webb, 1998). These rapid movements may allow animals to cope with sudden changes in flow regime. In the absence of M-cell firing, escape responses can occur through activation of other homologous reticulospinal neurons, but typically exhibit a slower reaction time (i.e. latency) and kinematic output [e.g. turning rate in stage 1 and distance covered during the response; Eaton et al. (2001)]. Whether differences in the flow conditions experienced by an individual throughout development may alter fast-start escape responses of individual fish or fish schools through phenotypic plasticity or selection remains unknown (Binning et al., 2014, 2015).

Using schools of a gregarious coral reef fish, we investigated how native water flow regimes experienced at the school's home reef affected school swimming behaviour and individual escape performance. We hypothesized that, when tested under the same flow conditions in the laboratory, schools collected from higher flow habitats would exhibit more cohesive and coordinated (i.e. aligned) swimming behaviour. We expect this more effective swimming pattern to aid in eliciting faster and more agile escape responses in individuals from higher flow reefs, due to a combination of phenotypic plasticity to changing environmental conditions and stronger selective pressures in habitats with greater flow.

RESULTS

A subset of 11 wild schools of the tropical damselfish species Chromis viridis were collected from seven shallow reef sites in the Lizard Island lagoon, northern Great Barrier Reef, Australia (Fig. 1A). Water flow speed was measured on five separate days and differed significantly among sites (LMM: F6,24=3.35, P=0.0154; Fig. 1B). In particular, sites 4 and 5 exhibited a significantly higher water flow speed than sites 1, 2, 3, 6 and 7 (Tukey's test, P<0.05). In addition, flow speeds were more variable at sites 4 and 5 (Fig. 1C).

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

Water flow speed. (A) Map of the seven collection sites. (B) Mean water flow speed at each of the collection sites (±s.e.m.). Grey circles indicate values in 2 cm/s bins. (C) Coefficient of variation in water flow at each of the collection sites.

School swimming and escape behaviour were assessed in a swim tunnel flowing at a slow speed [3.2 cm s−1 or ∼1 body length (L) s−1; Fig. 2] both before and after an aerial predator stimulus. P-values were corrected for type I error using a false detection rate (FDR) multiple test correction [pcutoff=0.0394; Benjamini and Hochberg (1995)]. Pre-stimulus behaviour was video-recorded at 30 frames per second (fps) and post-stimulus behaviour was recorded in high speed at 240 fps. School cohesion (as measured through nearest neighbour distance, NND) was not affected by either water flow speed from the school's home reef or time in relation to the stimulus [linear mixed-effects model (LMM) for flow: F1,9=0.21, P=0.6566; LMM for time: F1,691=1.42, P=0.2344; Fig. 3A]. School alignment was also not affected by water flow speed (LMM: F1,9=0.00, P=0.9891) but did vary significantly with time (LMM: F1,691=18.87, P<0.0001) (Fig. 3B). The interaction between water flow speed and time was not significant for either NND or alignment (P>pcutoff).

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

Diagram illustrating the swim tunnel experimental arena.

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

School swimming performance. (A) Nearest neighbour distance (NND) and (B) variability in individual alignment, before (−) and after (+) the stimulus. Bars are mean±s.e.m. Circles indicate data for individual fish in (A) 5 mm bins and (B) 5° bins. White circles and bars indicate data from low flow sites (n=56 fish) and grey circles and bars indicate data from high flow sites (n=32 fish).

Individual escape performance was assessed for latency, average turning rate during stage 1 of the reaction, and distance covered during the reaction (as a proxy for swimming speed). Individuals from high flow regime reefs exhibited greater escape performance, when compared with those collected from lower flow sites. There was a trend for latency in fish from high flow sites to be lower (indicating a faster reaction time) than in individuals from low flow sites, though this effect was not significant (Fig. 4A; LMM: F1,5=4.46, P=0.0883). In fish from higher flow sites, average turning rate was significantly greater than in fish from lower flow sites (Fig. 4B; LMM: F1,5=11.56, P=0.0193), suggesting that those fish from high flow sites exhibited a faster muscle contraction rate during stage 1 than those accustomed to lower flow. Distance covered was also significantly greater in fish from high flow sites than in fish from low flow sites (Fig. 4C; LMM: F1,5=7.67, P=0.0394), indicating that those fish swam a further distance with their escape response.

Fig. 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 4.

Individual fast-start performance. Individual fast-start performance according to high (grey dots with grey trend line, n=32 fish) or low (white dots with black trend line, n=56 fish) water flow regimes, including (A) latency (ms; log y-axis), (B) distance covered (mm) and (C) average turning rate (°/s).

DISCUSSION

Our study suggests that while school swimming behaviour is maintained across a range of localised water flow conditions, higher and more variable relative water flow speeds are associated with differences in individual fast-start performance. Water flow regime is a major driver in the distribution and abundance of behavioural, physiological and functional traits in fish assemblages in a range of systems (Binning et al., 2015; Fulton and Bellwood, 2005; McGuigan et al., 2003; Sinclair et al., 2014). As fast-start responses are modulated by a range of sensory and neural processes as well as anaerobic swimming performance, flow is likely inducing change on a range of functional levels within individual fish through plasticity and/or selection. These findings therefore indicate that an individual's performance may be tailored to the prevailing conditions at their home reef.

The effect of water flow speed on fast-start escape performance could occur as a result of a variety of mechanisms. Plasticity in behavioural traits in response to environmental factors has previously been demonstrated in response to water flow speed. Sinclair et al. (2014) observed increased boldness and aggression in the mosquitofish Gambusia holbrooki acclimated to a high flow environment when compared to those accustomed to low flow conditions. Any factor that increases the intensity and frequency of exercise in resident fishes may create a training effect that leads to improved physiological and behavioural performance (Anttila et al., 2011; Davison, 1997; Killen et al., 2016). A number of controlled laboratory studies have measured a training effect of water flow speed on aerobic metabolism and swimming performance, and found greater maximum metabolic rate, gait transition speed and critical swimming speed (Binning et al., 2015; Sinclair et al., 2014). Our results indicate that fast-start escape performance is likely subject to a similar training effect under high flow conditions, due to plasticity in anaerobic swimming performance, sensory systems and/or neural control. In humans, resistance training for both strength and endurance significantly improves anaerobic performance (Balabinis et al., 2003), suggesting that fish may also exhibit greater anaerobic swimming performance when they develop in high flow conditions. In addition, plasticity in the response to sensory cues occurs throughout ontogeny in a range of fish species in response to variable habitat conditions, by compensating for reduced cues in one sense with heightened sensitivity in other components of the sensory system (Chapman et al., 2010). Previous studies have also illustrated the scope for plasticity in fish neural activity, in M-cells in particular (Ebbesson and Braithwaite, 2012; Korn and Faber, 2005). Much of this plasticity in M-cell activity can be attributed to the varying sensitivity of neuromodulators to environmental conditions, which are integral in facilitating the transition from swimming to escape motor neurons (Song et al., 2015; Yeh et al., 1996). Variation in these neuromodulators may therefore be occurring in response to environmental flow conditions, consequently altering the escape neuron circuits' responsiveness.

Fast-start escape performance could also vary due to differences in selective pressure between high and low flow regimes (Higham et al., 2015). Previous studies have illustrated differential survival between fishes with varying locomotor performance (Swain, 1992). However, behavioural phenotypes may not experience a uniform degree of selective pressure across habitat types. For instance, slower performing individuals may experience stronger selective pressure in high flow compared to low flow regimes. This could account for the lower incidence of fish with ‘slow’ fast-start reactions than in schools collected from high flow regime reefs. In a study by Fu (2015), qingbo carp Spinibarbus sinensis exhibited a lower mortality rate when they had been acclimated to a high flow environment compared to those acclimated to still water. In addition, various studies suggest that water flow may reduce the ability of the lateral line to detect perturbations in the water created by attacking predators (Anwar et al., 2016; Feitl et al., 2010; Liao, 2006), potentially creating selection for individuals with a lower response threshold under high flow conditions. Further studies on how flow impacts predator strike performance and success would aid in understanding the contribution of selection to the distribution of fast-start phenotypes among habitat types.

Unlike fast-start performance of individual fish, there was no influence of water flow regime on school swimming behaviour. Previous studies have illustrated the plasticity of school cohesion and coordination in response to biotic and abiotic cues (Chivers et al., 1995; Cook et al., 2014; Domenici et al., 2002; Sogard and Olla, 1997; Webster et al., 2007; Weetman et al., 1998). Under an acute high flow treatment, Chicoli et al. (2014) found that schools of the giant danio Devario aequipinnatus exhibited increased alignment, orienting upstream into the flow. However, the results of this study suggest that this acute effect of flow on school structure does not translate into longer lasting effects on the school's behaviour when swimming at slower speeds.

Determining root causes of phenotypic divergence in wild-caught animals can be complicated by the difficulty in characterising all factors that may possibly influence results. Although we found differences in the relative water flow conditions among our study reefs, it is possible that additional factors that are correlated with water flow (e.g. predator density or behaviour, food availability, non-random sorting of individuals among groups) could be influencing the observed trends in escape behaviour (Fu et al., 2013; Killen et al., 2017; Marras and Domenici, 2013; Yan et al., 2015). Despite these potential disadvantages of characterising the distribution of behavioural phenotypes among animals from diverse natural environments, these types of studies are essential to place laboratory-based results in an ecological context and to understand real-world processes. Future work should aim to better understand which abiotic (e.g. turbulence) and biotic (e.g. food availability) factors associated with water flow drive the observed plasticity in escape behaviour. In addition, although it is potentially possible that the flow in the swim tunnel could affect the sensitivity of fish to mechano-acoustic stimuli, the flow in our set up was relatively slow (3.2 cm s−1). Therefore, any interference of the flow with the stimulation is likely to be minimal. We also controlled for the location of the school in reference to the stimulus (i.e. waited until six of the eight fish were in the box in the centre of the flow chamber) in order to minimise any effect of variability in school position on stimulus perception.

In complex habitats like coral reefs, changes in bathymetry due to disturbances such as storms can drastically alter flow conditions (Lilley and Schiel, 2006; Madin and Connolly, 2006). The results presented here suggest that a fish's defensive behaviour is tailored to its ambient environmental conditions. Therefore, it is possible that acute high flow events in coral reef habitats could create major challenges for fish assemblages in the future (Fulton and Bellwood, 2005; Munks et al., 2015; Nunes et al., 2013).

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Fish collection and maintenance

A subset of eleven wild schools of the tropical damselfish species C. viridis (standard length: 3.45±0.03 cm, body mass: 1.72±0.04 g, mean±s.e.) were collected from seven shallow reef sites (1.8–4 m depth; Fig. 1A) in the Lizard Island lagoon, northern Great Barrier Reef, Australia (14°40′08″S; 145°27′34″E). One to two schools were collected from each site, depending on the number of distinct schools found living on a given reef (not all sites contained multiple distinct schools, necessitating this design). Within sites, schools were separated by a minimum of 50 m and sites were separated by 400–3000 m. Fish were collected using hand nets and barrier nets and transported to the Lizard Island Research Station (LIRS). At the laboratory, fish were placed into experimental schools composed of eight individuals from their original schools and housed in replicate 20 l aquaria in a flow-through seawater system. Variation in body size within (<0.5 cm range) and among experimental schools (range=2.9–3.7 cm SL) was minimised (low flow average SL=3.31 cm, high flow average SL=3.47 cm).

C. viridis is an abundant, live coral-associated schooling species found on coral reefs throughout the Indo-Pacific region in groups ranging in size from three to hundreds of individuals (Nadler et al., 2014; Öhman et al., 1998; Pratchett et al., 2012). Information from a mark-recapture study indicated that C. viridis exhibit high site fidelity, with 64% of individuals found within 3 m of their home coral colony upon recapture three weeks later. Those individuals that had migrated >3 m from their home coral were found within an average of 34 m from their home coral colony, over a three-week period of calm weather (Nadler et al., unpublished data), suggesting that schools separated by a minimum of 50 m (as they were in this study) would be a part of distinct social groups. Fish were fed to satiation twice daily with INVE Aquaculture pellets and newly hatched Artemia sp. Laboratory tests of schooling characteristics and escape response were undertaken within seven days of capture, to avoid lab-induced changes in performance capacity.

Water flow measurement

Water flow speed was measured at each of the seven collection sites on five separate days under varying wind and weather conditions, to determine relative differences in flow between sites. Measurements were always taken at high tide (±1 h). Flow speed was determined using a precision vane-wheel flow meter (Hontzsch Gmbh, Waiblingen, Germany) placed approximately 1.25 m below the water surface. As C. viridis forages on plankton in the water column above the reef throughout the day (Coughlin and Strickler, 1990; Smith and Warburton, 1992), this depth would be a realistic indicator of the flow conditions experienced by these schools during processes that require swimming (particularly foraging). Measures of flow speed (cm s−1) were logged at 1 Hz for 180 s. An overall mean flow speed was then calculated for each site using data from all five days.

A previous study at Lizard Island that found that water flow speed at shallow, sheltered reef sites (comparable to those used here) were dictated primarily by wind conditions. Particularly, sheltered sites that are <3 m in depth did not exhibit significant variability until the wind speeds exceeded 15 knots (Johansen, 2014). All measures of water flow speed were taken at <3 m depth and under wind speeds of <15 knots, allowing us to look at a relative measure of flow speed among our sites using this methodology.

Swimming behaviour and escape response

Trials were conducted in a custom-built laminar flow swim tunnel (50 cm length×40 cm width×9 cm height; Fig. 2). This device allowed schools to swim in non-turbulent conditions at a slow uniform swim speed of approximately one L s−1 (3.2 cm s−1) for all trials, which mimics natural flow speed conditions at the seven collection sites on a calm day (Johansen, 2014). This low flow speed encouraged schools to swim but was slow enough that the fish were able to swim in any orientation to the flow (Nadler, personal observation). Seawater in the system was maintained at the ambient temperature for the study period (27–29°C). Experimental schools were placed in the swim tunnel and allowed to acclimate for 4 h. Afterwards, school swimming behaviour was video-recorded from below prior to the stimulus for 15 min (30 fps; Canon Powershot D10), using a mirror placed at a 45° angle. Escape responses were elicited using a standardised threat protocol in which a mechanical stimulus is dropped from above the experimental arena (Domenici et al., 2015). This stimulus was a black cylindrical object (2.5 cm diameter×12 cm length, 37.0 g) with a tapered end (to minimise surface waves), suspended 137 cm over the surface of the water in the swim tunnel. To avoid visual cues prior to the stimulus reaching the water's surface and to allow measuring response latency, this object was dropped through a white PVC pipe that ended immediately before it broke the water's surface (Domenici et al., 2008; Turesson and Domenici, 2007). A thread connecting the stimulus to the release point prevented it from touching the bottom of the tank (Domenici et al., 2008; Turesson and Domenici, 2007). As previous studies suggest that the school's alignment during an escape response is greatest with lateral stimulation at an angle of 30–120° (Marras et al., 2012), identical stimuli were placed 2 cm from each of the lateral walls in the centre of the swim tunnel. To control for a stimulus side preference, the use of the left or right lateral stimulus was alternated between trials. These stimuli remained suspended above the swim tunnel for the duration of the acclimation period using an electromagnet. Following the acclimation period, the stimulus was released using a switch, once a minimum of six of the eight fish were >3.5 cm from any wall of the swim tunnel and <4 L from the stimulus. This criterion aided in reducing the constraining effects that the walls of the swim tunnel may exert on an individuals' escape response and controlled for differences in escape performance that can occur with varying distance from the stimulus (Eaton and Emberley, 1991; Domenici and Batty, 1994, 1997). Each school's escape response was video-recorded from below in high speed (240fps; Casio Exilim HS EX-ZR1000). The swim tunnel was illuminated from above through a light diffusing filter using two 500 W spotlights.

Kinematic analysis

Videos were analysed using the ImageJ software (v 1.42). School swimming behaviour before and after the stimulus as well as individual fast-start performance attributes were examined as defined below.

School swimming behaviour

School swimming behaviour was characterised in terms of (1) school cohesion (nearest neighbour distance) and (2) alignment. (1) Nearest neighbour distance (NND): distance to the closest neighbour for each fish within the school, as measured by the distance from each fish’s centre of mass when stretched straight (CM). The location of the CM in video footage was measured as 0.35 L posterior of the snout, based on previous measurements of generalist fish species (Webb, 1978). The location of the fish’s CM when stretched straight is a useful point of measurement in fast-start studies on generalist fishes because of its strong ecological relevance for predator-prey interactions (Webb and Skadsen, 1980). (2) Alignment: the variation in the orientation of all school members to the horizontal (corresponding to the direction of flow; 180°, facing into the flow towards the front of the tank; 0°, oriented with the flow towards the back of the tank). As alignment angles spanned up to 360°, circular statistics were employed to find the school’s mean orientation (Bachelet, 1981), as calculated in the software Oriana 4 (Kovach Computing Services, Anglesey, Wales). In order to assess the alignment of each individual to their schoolmates’ orientation, alignment was calculated as the angular difference (in degrees) between each individual’s orientation and the school’s mean orientation. From the 15 min pre-stimulus video recording, five frames (one frame every 3 min for the duration of the recording) were analysed for each of the characteristics outlined below. In addition, school escape response variables were assessed at three times post-stimulus from the high-speed video recording (0, 20 and 100 ms post-stimulus). The stimulus onset was defined as the frame at which the stimulus first touched the water's surface (indicated by time=0 ms), and illustrates the school's behaviour immediately preceding stimulation. The remaining times (20 ms and 100 ms post-stimulus) were chosen because approximately one-third and two-thirds of fish in each school exhibited latencies for within each of those times respectively. Therefore, these times illustrate the school's behaviour early and late in the school's response to the threat stimulus.

Individual fast-start performance

Individual escape performance was characterised through (1) response latency, (2) average turning rate and (3) distance covered. Previous studies suggest that the (4) stimulus distance can influence latency, average turning rate and distance covered (Domenici and Batty 1994, 1997), so this measure was included as a covariate in the analysis. (1) Response latency: the time period between the stimulus onset (contact with the water surface) and the fish's onset of the escape response. (2) Average turning rate: calculated by dividing the stage 1 angle [the angle between the lines intersecting the head and CM at the start and end of stage (1) by the duration of stage 1 (Domenici, 2004)]. Stage 1 is the stage immediately post-stimulus, in which fish contract the muscles on one side of their body, causing the fish to bend into a C shape. (3) Distance covered: distance that the fish's CM travelled within the first 10 frames (i.e. 42 ms) of their reaction. This duration was determined using a preliminary analysis, in which the average duration of stages 1 and 2 were calculated for the escape response of 24 individuals (i.e. one random fish per trial). This short time frame was used as a proxy for mean swimming speed in order to avoid issues with wall effects. Individuals less than 2 L from any wall of the swim tunnel at the time of their response were excluded from this analysis [10% of total; Eaton and Emberley (1991)]. (4) Stimulus distance: distance from the stimulus to the fish's CM.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were conducted in the R Statistical Environment v3.2.4 (R Development Core Team, 2016), using the packages ‘nlme’ and ‘multcomp’ (Pinheiro et al., 2016; Torsten et al., 2008). Residual and quantile-quantile plots were assessed for each model in order to ensure that all assumptions were met. To meet model assumptions, water flow speed and latency were log-transformed while NND and alignment were square-root transformed. Differences in water flow speed among sites were assessed using a linear mixed-effects model (LMM), with site as a fixed effect and sampling date as a random effect to account for differences in conditions among measuring days. For this analysis, flow was analysed as a continuous variable. Tukey's HSD post-hoc tests were used to further investigate significant differences between sites detected by the LMM.

For all remaining analyses, flow was analysed as a categorical variable (low flow=8.2–10 m/s; high flow=21.8 m/s). P-values were corrected for type I error using a false detection rate (FDR) multiple test correction [pcutoff=0.0394; Benjamini and Hochberg (1995)]. The influence of water flow speed on school swimming behaviour (NND and alignment) was tested using a LMM with water flow speed and time in relation to the stimulus (and their interaction) as fixed effects, with site, school and individual as random effects. Individual fast-start performance (latency, average turning rate and distance covered) was examined using a LMM with flow speed as a fixed effect and school and site as random effects. Stimulus distance was included as a covariate in this analysis. The R code used for this analysis has been included as electronic supplementary material.

Acknowledgements

We thank the Lizard Island Research Station staff, Ross Barrett, Katherine Corkill, Rahel Zemoi and Stephen Brown for logistical support, Rhondda Jones for statistical advice and two reviewers for helpful comments that greatly improved the manuscript. This research was conducted under James Cook University Animal Ethics approval number A2103.

Footnotes

  • Competing interests

    The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

  • Author contributions

    Conceptualization: L.E.N., S.S.K., P.D., M.I.M.; Methodology: L.E.N., S.S.K., P.D., M.I.M.; Software: L.E.N.; Validation: L.E.N.; Formal analysis: L.E.N., S.S.K., P.D., M.I.M.; Investigation: L.E.N.; Resources: L.E.N., S.S.K.; Data curation: L.E.N.; Writing - original draft: L.E.N.; Writing - review & editing: L.E.N., S.S.K., P.D., M.I.M.; Visualization: L.E.N.; Supervision: P.D., M.I.M.; Project administration: L.E.N.; Funding acquisition: L.E.N., S.S.K., M.I.M.

  • Funding

    Funding was provided by an Australian Postgraduate Award, International Postgraduate Research Scholarship, Lizard Island Reef Research Foundation Doctoral Fellowship, Great Barrier Reef Marine Park Authority Science for Management Award and James Cook University Graduate Research Scheme to L.E.N., a Natural Environment Research Council Advanced Fellowship [NE/J019100/1] to S.S.K., Australian Research Council Discovery Grant [DP170103372] to M.I.M. and ARC Centre of Excellence for Coral Reef Studies funding [EI140100117] to M.I.M.

  • Supplementary information

    Supplementary information available online at http://bio.biologists.org/lookup/doi/10.1242/bio.031997.supplemental

  • Data availability

    The data from this study are available in the Supplementary Material.

  • Received December 15, 2017.
  • Accepted September 3, 2018.
  • © 2018. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Abrahams, M. V. and
    2. Colgan, P. W.
    (1985). Risk of predation, hydrodynamic efficiency and their influence on school structure. Env. Biol. Fish. 13, 195-202. doi:10.1007/BF00000931
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  2. ↵
    1. Anttila, K.,
    2. Jokikokko, E.,
    3. Erkinaro, J.,
    4. Järvilehto, M. and
    5. Mänttäri, S.
    (2011). Effects of training on functional variables of muscles in reared Atlantic salmon Salmo salar smolts: connection to downstream migration pattern. J. Fish Biol. 78, 552-566. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2010.02871.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Anwar, S. B.,
    2. Cathcart, K.,
    3. Darakananda, K.,
    4. Gaing, A. N.,
    5. Shin, S. Y.,
    6. Vronay, X.,
    7. Wright, D. N. and
    8. Ellerby, D. J.
    (2016). The effects of steady swimming on fish escape performance. J. Comp. Physiol. A 202, 425-433. doi:10.1007/s00359-016-1090-3
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  4. ↵
    1. Bachelet, E.
    (1981). Circular Statistics in Biology. London: Academic Press.
  5. ↵
    1. Balabinis, C. P.,
    2. Psarakis, C. H.,
    3. Moukas, M.,
    4. Vassiliou, M. P. and
    5. Behrakis, P. K.
    (2003). Early phase changes by concurrent endurance and strength training. J. Strength Cond. Res. 17, 393. doi:10.1519/1533-4287(2003)017<0393:EPCBCE>2.0.CO;2
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  6. ↵
    1. Benjamini, Y. and
    2. Hochberg, Y.
    (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Series. B. Stat. Methodol. 57, 289-300.
    OpenUrl
  7. ↵
    1. Binning, S. A.,
    2. Roche, D. G. and
    3. Fulton, C. J.
    (2014). Localised intraspecific variation in the swimming phenotype of a coral reef fish across different wave exposures. Oecologia 174, 623-630. doi:10.1007/s00442-013-2794-5
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Binning, S. A.,
    2. Ros, A. F. H.,
    3. Nusbaumer, D. and
    4. Roche, D. G.
    (2015). Physiological plasticity to water flow habitat in the damselfish, Acanthochromis polyacanthus: Linking phenotype to performance. PLoS ONE 10, e0121983. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0121983
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Chapman, B. B.,
    2. Morrell, L. J.,
    3. Tosh, C. R. and
    4. Krause, J.
    (2010). Behavioural consequences of sensory plasticity in guppies. Proc. Biol. Sci. 277, 1395-1401. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.2055
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Chicoli, A.,
    2. Butail, S.,
    3. Lun, Y.,
    4. Bak-Coleman, J.,
    5. Coombs, S. and
    6. Paley, D. A.
    (2014). The effects of flow on schooling Devario aequipinnatus: School structure, startle response and information transmission. J. Fish Biol. 84, 1401-1421. doi:10.1111/jfb.12365
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Chivers, D. P.,
    2. Brown, G. E. and
    3. Smith, J. F. R.
    (1995). Familiarity and shoal cohesion in fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas): implications for antipredator behavior. Can. J. Zool. 73, 955-960. doi:10.1139/z95-111
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  12. ↵
    1. Cook, D. G.,
    2. Brown, E. J.,
    3. Lefevre, S.,
    4. Domenici, P. and
    5. Steffensen, J. F.
    (2014). The response of striped surfperch Embiotoca lateralis to progressive hypoxia: Swimming activity, shoal structure, and estimated metabolic expenditure. J Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 460, 162-169. doi:10.1016/j.jembe.2014.07.002
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  13. ↵
    1. Coughlin, D. J. and
    2. Strickler, J. R.
    (1990). Zooplankton capture by a coral reef fish: an adaptive response to evasive prey. Env. Biol. Fish. 29, 35-42. doi:10.1007/BF00000566
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  14. ↵
    1. Davison, W.
    (1997). The effects of exercise training on teleost fish, a review of recent literature. Comp. Biochem. Phys. A 117, 67-75. doi:10.1016/S0300-9629(96)00284-8
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  15. ↵
    1. Domenici, P.
    (2004). Escape manoeuvres in the spiny dogfish (Squalus acanthias). J. Exp. Biol. 207, 2339-2349. doi:10.1242/jeb.01015
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    1. Domenici, P.
    (2010). Context-dependent variability in the components of fish escape response: integrating locomotor performance and behavior. J. Exp. Zool. A 313A, 59-79. doi:10.1002/jez.580
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Domenici, P. and
    2. Batty, R. S.
    (1994). Escape maneoeuvres of schooling Clupea harengus. J. Fish Biol. 45, 97-110. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.1994.tb01086.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  18. ↵
    1. Domenici, P. and
    2. Batty, R. S.
    (1997). Escape behaviour of solitary herring (Clupea harengus) and comparisons with schooling individuals. Mar. Biol. 128, 29-38. doi:10.1007/s002270050065
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  19. ↵
    1. Domenici, P. and
    2. Blake, R. W.
    (1997). The kinematics and performance of fish fast-start swimming. J. Exp. Biol. 200, 1165-1178.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    1. Domenici, P.,
    2. Ferrari, R. S.,
    3. Steffensen, J. F. and
    4. Batty, R. S.
    (2002). The effect of progressive hypoxia on school structure and dynamics in Atlantic herring Clupea harengus. Proc. Biol. Sci. 269, 2103-2111. doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.2107
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. Domenici, P.,
    2. Turesson, H.,
    3. Brodersen, J. and
    4. Bronmark, C.
    (2008). Predator-induced morphology enhances escape locomotion in crucian carp. Proc. Biol. Sci 275, 195-201. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.1088
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Domenici, P.,
    2. Norin, T.,
    3. Bushnell, P. G.,
    4. Johansen, J. L.,
    5. Skov, P. V.,
    6. Svendsen, M. B. S.,
    7. Steffensen, J. F. and
    8. Abe, A. S.
    (2015). Fast-starting after a breath: air-breathing motions are kinematically similar to escape responses in the catfish Hoplosternum littorale. Biol. Open 4, 79-85. doi:10.1242/bio.20149332
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    1. Eaton, R. C. and
    2. Emberley, D.
    (1991). How stimulus direction determines the trajectory of the Mauthner-initiated escape response in a teleost fish. J. Exp. Biol. 161, 469-487.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  24. ↵
    1. Eaton, R. C.,
    2. Lee, R. K. K. and
    3. Foreman, M. B.
    (2001). The Mauthner cell and other identified neurons of the brainstem escape network of fish. Prog. Neurobiol. 63, 467-485. doi:10.1016/S0301-0082(00)00047-2
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  25. ↵
    1. Ebbesson, L. O. E. and
    2. Braithwaite, V. A.
    (2012). Environmental effects on fish neural plasticity and cognition. J. Fish Biol. 81, 2151-2174. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2012.03486.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Feitl, K. E.,
    2. Ngo, V. and
    3. McHenry, M. J.
    (2010). Are fish less responsive to a flow stimulus when swimming? J. Exp Biol. 213, 3131-3137. doi:10.1242/jeb.045518
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  27. ↵
    1. Fu, S.-J.
    (2015). Flow and stress acclimation both enhance predator avoidance in a common cyprinid fish. Aq. Biol. 24, 1-8. doi:10.3354/ab00633
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  28. ↵
    1. Fu, S.-J.,
    2. Cao, Z.-D.,
    3. Yan, G.-J.,
    4. Fu, C. and
    5. Pang, X.
    (2013). Integrating environmental variation, predation pressure, phenotypic plasticity and locomotor performance. Oecologia 173, 343-354. doi:10.1007/s00442-013-2626-7
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Fulton, C. J. and
    2. Bellwood, D. R.
    (2005). Wave-induced water motion and the functional implications for coral reef fish assemblages. Limnol. Oceanogr. 50, 255-264. doi:10.4319/lo.2005.50.1.0255
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  30. ↵
    1. Handegard, N. O.,
    2. Boswell, K. M.,
    3. Ioannou, C. C.,
    4. Leblanc, S. P.,
    5. Tjøstheim, D. B. and
    6. Couzin, I. D.
    (2012). The dynamics of coordinated group hunting and collective information transfer among schooling prey. Curr. Biol. 22, 1213-1217. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.04.050
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Hansen, M. J.,
    2. Schaerf, T. M. and
    3. Ward, A. J. W.
    (2015). The influence of nutritional state on individual and group movement behaviour in shoals of crimson-spotted rainbowfish (Melanotaenia duboulayi). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 69, 1713-1722. doi:10.1007/s00265-015-1983-0
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  32. ↵
    1. Herskin, J. and
    2. Steffensen, J. F.
    (1998). Energy savings in sea bass swimming in a school: measurements of tail beat frequency and oxygen consumption at different swimming speeds. J. Fish Biol. 53, 366-376. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.1998.tb00986.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  33. ↵
    1. Higham, T. E.,
    2. Stewart, W. J. and
    3. Wainwright, P. C.
    (2015). Turbulence, temperature, and turbidity: the ecomechanics of predator-prey interactions in fishes. Integr. Comp. Biol. 55, 6-20. doi:10.1093/icb/icv052
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Huntington, T. G.
    (2006). Evidence for intensification of the global water cycle: Review and synthesis. J. Hydro. 319, 83-95. doi:10.1016/j.jhydrol.2005.07.003
    OpenUrlCrossRefGeoRefWeb of Science
  35. ↵
    1. Johansen, J. L.
    (2014). Quantifying water flow within aquatic ecosystems using load cell sensors: a profile of currents experienced by coral reef organisms around Lizard Island, Great Barrier Reef, Australia. PLoS ONE 9, e83240. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0083240
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Johansen, J. L.,
    2. Fulton, C. J. and
    3. Bellwood, D. R.
    (2007). Avoiding the flow: refuges expand the swimming potential of coral reef fishes. Coral Reefs 26, 577-583. doi:10.1007/s00338-007-0217-y
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  37. ↵
    1. Killen, S. S.,
    2. Marras, S.,
    3. Steffensen, J. F. and
    4. McKenzie, D. J.
    (2012). Aerobic capacity influences the spatial position of individuals within fish schools. Proc. Biol. Sci. 279, 357-364. doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.1006
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Killen, S. S.,
    2. Croft, D. P.,
    3. Salin, K.,
    4. Darden, S. K. and
    5. Higham, T.
    (2016). Male sexually coercive behaviour drives increased swimming efficiency in female guppies. Funct. Ecol. 30, 576-583. doi:10.1111/1365-2435.12527
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  39. ↵
    1. Killen, S. S.,
    2. Marras, S.,
    3. Nadler, L. E. and
    4. Domenici, P.
    (2017). The role of physiological traits in assortment among and within fish shoals. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 372, 20160233. doi:10.1098/rstb.2016.0233
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    1. Korn, H. and
    2. Faber, D. S.
    (2005). The Mauthner cell half a century later: a neurobiological model for decision-making? Neuron 47, 13-28. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.019
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  41. ↵
    1. Krause, J. and
    2. Ruxton, G. D.
    (2002). Living in Groups. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  42. ↵
    1. Krupczynski, P. and
    2. Schuster, S.
    (2008). Fruit-catching fish tune their fast starts to compensate for drift. Curr. Biol. 18, 1961-1965. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.10.066
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Langerhans, R. B.
    (2008). Predictability of phenotypic differentiation across flow regimes in fishes. Integr. Comp. Biol. 48, 750-768. doi:10.1093/icb/icn092
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Liao, J. C.
    (2006). The role of the lateral line and vision on body kinematics and hydrodynamic preference of rainbow trout in turbulent flow. J. Exp. Biol. 209, 4077-4090. doi:10.1242/jeb.02487
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. ↵
    1. Liao, J. C.
    (2007). A review of fish swimming mechanics and behaviour in altered flows. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 362, 1973-1993. doi:10.1098/rstb.2007.2082
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  46. ↵
    1. Lilley, S. A. and
    2. Schiel, D. R.
    (2006). Community effects following the deletion of a habitat-forming alga from rocky marine shores. Oecologia 148, 672-681. doi:10.1007/s00442-006-0411-6
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Madin, J. S. and
    2. Connolly, S. R.
    (2006). Ecological consequences of major hydrodynamic disturbances on coral reefs. Nature 444, 477-480. doi:10.1038/nature05328
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  48. ↵
    1. Madin, J. S.,
    2. Black, K. P. and
    3. Connolly, S. R.
    (2006). Scaling water motion on coral reefs: from regional to organismal scales. Coral Reefs 25, 635-644. doi:10.1007/s00338-006-0137-2
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  49. ↵
    1. Marras, S. and
    2. Domenici, P.
    (2013). Schooling fish under attack are not all equal: Some lead, others follow. PLoS ONE 8, e65784. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0065784
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Marras, S.,
    2. Batty, R. S. and
    3. Domenici, P.
    (2012). Information transfer and antipredator maneuvers in schooling herring. Adap. Behav. 20, 44-56. doi:10.1177/1059712311426799
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  51. ↵
    1. Marras, S.,
    2. Killen, S. S.,
    3. Lindström, J.,
    4. McKenzie, D. J.,
    5. Steffensen, J. F. and
    6. Domenici, P.
    (2015). Fish swimming in schools save energy regardless of their spatial position. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 69, 219-226. doi:10.1007/s00265-014-1834-4
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. McGuigan, K.,
    2. Franklin, C. E.,
    3. Moritz, C. and
    4. Blows, M. W.
    (2003). Adaptation of rainbow fish to lake and stream habitats. Evolution 57, 104. doi:10.1111/j.0014-3820.2003.tb00219.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  53. ↵
    1. McLaren, J. D.,
    2. Shamoun-Baranes, J.,
    3. Dokter, A. M.,
    4. Klaassen, R. H. and
    5. Bouten, W.
    (2014). Optimal orientation in flows: providing a benchmark for animal movement strategies. J. R. Soc. Interface 11, 20140588. doi:10.1098/rsif.2014.0588
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  54. ↵
    1. Munks, L. S.,
    2. Harvey, E. S. and
    3. Saunders, B. J.
    (2015). Storm-induced changes in environmental conditions are correlated with shifts in temperate reef fish abundance and diversity. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 472, 77-88. doi:10.1016/j.jembe.2015.06.006
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  55. ↵
    1. Nadler, L. E.,
    2. McNeill, D. C.,
    3. Alwany, M. A. and
    4. Bailey, D. M.
    (2014). Effect of habitat characteristics on the distribution and abundance of damselfish within a Red Sea reef. Env. Biol. Fish. 97, 1265-1277. doi:10.1007/s10641-013-0212-9
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  56. ↵
    1. Nadler, L. E.,
    2. Killen, S. S.,
    3. McClure, E. C.,
    4. Munday, P. L. and
    5. McCormick, M. I.
    (2016). Shoaling reduces metabolic rate in a gregarious coral reef fish species. J. Exp. Biol. 219, 2802-2805. doi:10.1242/jeb.139493
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    1. Nikora, V.
    (2010). Hydrodynamics of aquatic ecosystems: an interface between ecology, biomechanics and environmental fluid mechanics. River Res. Appl. 26, 367-384. doi:10.1002/rra.1291
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  58. ↵
    1. Nunes, J. A. C. C.,
    2. Sampaio, C. L. S. and
    3. Barros, F.
    (2013). How wave exposure, group size and habitat complexity influence foraging and population densities in fishes of the genus Halichoeres (Perciformes: Labridae) on tropical rocky shores. Mar. Biol. 160, 2383-2394. doi:10.1007/s00227-013-2233-5
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  59. ↵
    1. Öhman, M. C.,
    2. Munday, P. L.,
    3. Jones, G. P. and
    4. Caley, M. J.
    (1998). Settlement strategies and distribution patterns of coral-reef fishes. J Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 225, 219-238. doi:10.1016/S0022-0981(97)00224-4
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  60. ↵
    1. Olson, K. R.
    (1998). The cardiovascular system. In The Physiology of Fishes (ed. D. H. Evans), pp. 129-154. Boca Raton: CRC Press.
  61. ↵
    1. Pinheiro, J.,
    2. Bates, D.,
    3. DebRoy, S.,
    4. Sarkar, D. and
    5. Team, R. C. D.
    (2016). nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. In R package version 3.1-122.
  62. ↵
    1. Poff, N. L. R.,
    2. Allan, J. D.,
    3. Bain, M. B.,
    4. Karr, J. R.,
    5. Prestegaard, K. L.,
    6. Richter, B. D.,
    7. Sparks, R. E. and
    8. Stromberg, J. C.
    (1997). The natural flow regime. Bioscience 47, 769-784. doi:10.2307/1313099
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  63. ↵
    1. Pratchett, M. S.,
    2. Coker, D. J.,
    3. Jones, G. P. and
    4. Munday, P. L.
    (2012). Specialization in habitat use by coral reef damselfishes and their susceptibility to habitat loss. Ecol. Evol. 2, 2168-2180. doi:10.1002/ece3.321
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    R Development Core Team. (2016). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
  65. ↵
    1. Riley, J. R.,
    2. Reynolds, D. R.,
    3. Smith, A. D.,
    4. Edwards, A. S.,
    5. Osborne, J. L.,
    6. Williams, I. H. and
    7. McCartney, H. A.
    (1999). Compensation for wind drift by bumble-bees. Nature 400, 126. doi:10.1038/22029
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  66. ↵
    1. Shaw, E.
    (1978). Schooling fishes: the school, a truly egalitarian form of organization in which all membersof the group are alike in influence, offers substantial benefits to its participants. Am. Sci. 66, 166-175.
    OpenUrlWeb of Science
  67. ↵
    1. Sinclair, E. L. E.,
    2. de Souza, C. R. N.,
    3. Ward, A. J. W.,
    4. Seebacher, F. and
    5. Herrel, A.
    (2014). Exercise changes behaviour. Funct. Ecol. 28, 652-659. doi:10.1111/1365-2435.12198
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  68. ↵
    1. Smith, M. F. L. and
    2. Warburton, K.
    (1992). Predator shoaling moderates the confusion effect in blue-green chromis, Chromis viridis. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 30, 103-107. doi:10.1007/BF00173946
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  69. ↵
    1. Sogard, S. M. and
    2. Olla, B. L.
    (1997). The influence of hunger and predation risk on group cohesion in a pelagic fish, walleye pollock Theragra chalcogramma. Env. Biol. Fish. 50, 405-413. doi:10.1023/A:1007393307007
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  70. ↵
    1. Song, J.,
    2. Ampatzis, K.,
    3. Ausborn, J. and
    4. El Manira, A.
    (2015). A hardwired circuit supplemented with endocannabinoids encodes behavioral choice in zebrafish. Curr. Biol. 25, 2610-2620. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.08.042
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Srygley, R. B.
    (2001). Compensation for fluctuations in crosswind drift without stationary landmarks in butterflies migrating over seas. Anim. Behav. 61, 191-203. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1551
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  72. ↵
    1. Swain, D. P.
    (1992). The functional basis of natural selection for vertebral traits of larvae in the stickleback Gasterosteus aculeatus. Evolution 46, 987-997. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.1992.tb00614.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  73. ↵
    1. Thorup, K.,
    2. Alerstam, T.,
    3. Hake, M. and
    4. Kjellen, N.
    (2003). Bird orientation: compensation for wind drift in migrating raptors is age dependent. Proc. Biol. Sci. 270 Suppl. 1, S8-S11. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2003.0014
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  74. ↵
    1. Torsten, H.,
    2. Bretz, F. and
    3. Westfall, P.
    (2008). Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. Biomet. J. 50, 346-363. doi:10.1002/bimj.200810425
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  75. ↵
    1. Turesson, H. and
    2. Domenici, P.
    (2007). Escape latency is size independent in grey mullet. J. Fish Biol. 71, 253-259. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2007.01490.x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  76. ↵
    1. Tytell, E. D. and
    2. Lauder, G. V.
    (2008). Hydrodynamics of the escape response in bluegill sunfish, Lepomis macrochirus. J. Exp Biol. 211, 3359-3369. doi:10.1242/jeb.020917
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  77. ↵
    1. Wakeling, J. M.
    (2005). Fast-start mechanics. In Fish Physiology, vol. 23 (ed. R. E. Shadwick and G. V. Lauder), pp. 333-368. San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press Inc.
    OpenUrl
  78. ↵
    1. Ward, A. and
    2. Webster, M.
    (2016). Sociality: The Behaviour of Group-Living Animals. Switzerland: Springer.
  79. ↵
    1. Webb, P. W.
    (1978). Fast-start performance and body form in seven species of teleost fish. J. Exp. Biol. 74, 211-226.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  80. ↵
    1. Webb, P. W.
    (1998). Swimming. In The Physiology of Fishes (ed. D. H. Evans), pp. 3-24. Boca Raton: CRC Press.
  81. ↵
    1. Webb, P. W. and
    2. Skadsen, J. M.
    (1980). Strike tactics of Esox. Can. J. Zool. 58, 1462-1469. doi:10.1139/z80-201
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  82. ↵
    1. Webster, M. M.,
    2. Goldsmith, J.,
    3. Ward, A. J. W. and
    4. Hart, P. J. B.
    (2007). Habitat-specific chemical cues influence association preferences and shoal cohesion in fish. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 62, 273-280. doi:10.1007/s00265-007-0462-7
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  83. ↵
    1. Weetman, D.,
    2. Atkinson, D. and
    3. Chubb, J. C.
    (1998). Effects of temperature on anti-predator behaviour in the guppy Poecilia reticulata. Anim. Behav. 55, 1361-1372. doi:10.1006/anbe.1997.0666
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  84. ↵
    1. Weihs, D.
    (1973). Hydromechanics of fish schooling. Nature 241, 290-291. doi:10.1038/241290a0
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  85. ↵
    1. West-Eberhard, M. J.
    (1989). Phenotypic plasticity and the origins of diversity. Ann. Rev. Ecol. System. 20, 249-278. doi:10.1146/annurev.es.20.110189.001341
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  86. ↵
    1. Yan, G. J.,
    2. He, X. K.,
    3. Cao, Z. D. and
    4. Fu, S. J.
    (2015). Effects of fasting and feeding on the fast-start swimming performance of southern catfish Silurus meridionalis. J. Fish Biol. 86, 605-614. doi:10.1111/jfb.12595
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  87. ↵
    1. Yeh, S.-R.,
    2. Fricke, R. A. and
    3. Edwards, D. H.
    (1996). The effect of social experience on serotonergic modulation of the escape circuit of crayfish. Science 271, 366-369. doi:10.1126/science.271.5247.366
    OpenUrlAbstract
View Abstract
Previous ArticleNext Article
Back to top
Previous ArticleNext Article

This Issue

RSSRSS

Keywords

  • Schooling behaviour
  • Fast-start behaviour
  • Anaerobic capacity
  • Habitat
  • Escape response
  • Plasticity

 Download PDF

Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on Biology Open.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Role of water flow regime in the swimming behaviour and escape performance of a schooling fish
(Your Name) has sent you a message from Biology Open
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the Biology Open web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
Research Article
Role of water flow regime in the swimming behaviour and escape performance of a schooling fish
Lauren E. Nadler, Shaun S. Killen, Paolo Domenici, Mark I. McCormick
Biology Open 2018 7: bio031997 doi: 10.1242/bio.031997 Published 19 October 2018
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
Research Article
Role of water flow regime in the swimming behaviour and escape performance of a schooling fish
Lauren E. Nadler, Shaun S. Killen, Paolo Domenici, Mark I. McCormick
Biology Open 2018 7: bio031997 doi: 10.1242/bio.031997 Published 19 October 2018

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Alerts

Please log in to add an alert for this article.

Sign in to email alerts with your email address

Article Navigation

  • Top
  • Article
    • ABSTRACT
    • INTRODUCTION
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • MATERIALS AND METHODS
    • Acknowledgements
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & tables
  • Supp info
  • Info & metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF + SI
  • PDF

Related articles

Cited by...

More in this TOC section

  • Wound-induced polyploidization is dependent on Integrin-Yki signaling
  • A stem cell based in vitro model of NAFLD enables the analysis of patient specific individual metabolic adaptations in response to a high fat diet and AdipoRon interference
  • Mutations in the splicing regulator Prp31 lead to retinal degeneration in Drosophila
Show more RESEARCH ARTICLE

Similar articles

Other journals from The Company of Biologists

Development

Journal of Cell Science

Journal of Experimental Biology

Disease Models & Mechanisms

Advertisement

Biology Open and COVID-19

We are aware that the COVID-19 pandemic is having an unprecedented impact on researchers worldwide. The Editors of all The Company of Biologists’ journals have been considering ways in which we can alleviate concerns that members of our community may have around publishing activities during this time. Read about the actions we are taking at this time.

Please don’t hesitate to contact the Editorial Office if you have any questions or concerns.


2020 at The Company of Biologists

Despite 2020’s challenges, we achieved a lot at The Company of Biologists. In the midst of the pandemic, we have seen long-term projects and new ventures come to fruition. Read our full lowdown of 2020.


Interview- Sebastian Markert

Sebastian Markert is first author of a paper in BiO using C. elegans to model amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. In an interview, he talks about the potential implications of his work and his future plans.


Three communities to support biologists to everywhere

Online communities have never been more important. If you’re looking for somewhere to meet fellow scientists, take part in topical discussions and find virtual events in your field, take a look at each of our community sites:

  • The Node: the community site for and by developmental biologists
  • preLights: the preprint highlights service run by the biological community
  • FocalPlane: the community site for microscopists and biologists alike

Articles

  • Accepted manuscripts
  • Issue in progress
  • Latest complete issue
  • Issue archive
  • Archive by article type
  • Interviews
  • Sign up for alerts

About us

  • About BiO
  • Editors and Board
  • Editor biographies
  • Grants and funding
  • Journal Meetings
  • Workshops
  • The Company of Biologists

For Authors

  • Submit a manuscript
  • Aims and scope
  • Presubmission enquiries
  • Article types
  • Manuscript preparation
  • Cover suggestions
  • Editorial process
  • Promoting your paper
  • Open Access

Journal Info

  • Journal policies
  • Rights and permissions
  • Media policies
  • Reviewer guide
  • Sign up for alerts

Contact

  • Contact BiO
  • Advertising
  • Feedback

Twitter   YouTube   LinkedIn

© 2021   The Company of Biologists Ltd   Registered Charity 277992